The FNAME yields equivalent results differentiating amnestic mild cognitive impairment from subjective cognitive decline and healthy controls in Mexico and the Netherlands ; Neuropsychiatry and behavioral neurology/Mild cognitive impairment/Early symptomatic disease

Abstract Background The Face‐Name Associative Memory Exam (FNAME) has been shown to detect memory deficits early in the spectrum of Alzheimer’s disease. In this study, we used a newly developed and further extended version of the FNAME, to evaluate the discriminating ability of this test in amnestic cognitive decline (aMCI), amnestic subjective cognitive decline (aSMD), and healthy controls (HC) in two different cultures, namely Mexico and the Netherlands. Method Participants were recruited in two sites, in Mexico (n= 69) and the Netherlands (n= 55). The mean age of the subjects was 69.3 ±6 ye... Mehr ...

Verfasser: Flores‐Vazquez, Juan Francisco
Van der Kolk, Iris
Contreras‐López, José Juan
Cruz‐Contreras, Cecilia
Stegeman, Rutger A.
del Pilar Andrés‐Benito, María
Acosta‐Castillo, Gilberto Isaac
Aleman, André
Sosa‐Ortíz, Ana Luisa
Enriquez‐Geppert, Stefanie
Dokumenttyp: Artikel
Erscheinungsdatum: 2020
Reihe/Periodikum: Alzheimer's & Dementia ; volume 16, issue S6 ; ISSN 1552-5260 1552-5279
Verlag/Hrsg.: Wiley
Schlagwörter: Psychiatry and Mental health / Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience / Geriatrics and Gerontology / Neurology (clinical) / Developmental Neuroscience / Health Policy / Epidemiology
Sprache: Englisch
Permalink: https://search.fid-benelux.de/Record/base-26850878
Datenquelle: BASE; Originalkatalog
Powered By: BASE
Link(s) : http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/alz.044333

Abstract Background The Face‐Name Associative Memory Exam (FNAME) has been shown to detect memory deficits early in the spectrum of Alzheimer’s disease. In this study, we used a newly developed and further extended version of the FNAME, to evaluate the discriminating ability of this test in amnestic cognitive decline (aMCI), amnestic subjective cognitive decline (aSMD), and healthy controls (HC) in two different cultures, namely Mexico and the Netherlands. Method Participants were recruited in two sites, in Mexico (n= 69) and the Netherlands (n= 55). The mean age of the subjects was 69.3 ±6 years, and the mean years of education 15.5 ±5 years. In this study we used a newly extended version of the FNAME that was developed in both countries concurrently. A total of 42 aMCI, 43 aSCD, and 32 HC were assessed. We used a mixed model ANOVA, with the factors FNAME (7 subtests), GROUPS (aMCI, aSCD, HC), and the SITES (Mexico, the Netherlands) and post hoc t‐tests. Result Crucially, a high interaction of FNAME x GROUPS was revealed. Subsequent t–tests showed that the FNAME was able to detect differences between aMCI and aSCD/HC. No significant differences were observed between aSCD and HC. The three‐way interaction’s effect was non‐significant (F= 1.7, p= 0.07). As expected, we found a significant main effect of FNAME (F= 228.4, p< 0.001) demonstrating differences of demands of the subtests (e.g., recall, recognition), as well as a main effect of GROUPS (F= 2.1, p= 0.02). A two‐way interaction of FNAME x SITES was similarly revealed (F= 2.95, p< 0.001), reflecting a difference regarding the performance between sites. Conclusion This newly adapted and extended version of the FNAME test is able to differentiate aMCI subjects from aSCD and EC both in Mexico and in the Netherlands. Further studies should focus on better understanding the differences in performance in these countries.